My response to it exemplifies operant and not respondent behavior

My response to it exemplifies operant and not respondent behavior. Operant behavior is much more varied and less predictable, it operates on the environment to produce some effects (hence it is sometimes also referred to as “instrumental” behavior). The ensuing paradigms differed from classical conditioning studies in multiple ways. To produce operant

conditioning in the laboratory, a hungry animal would be placed in Inhibitors,research,lifescience,medical a box equipped with a protruding bar (“Skinner box”). A light above the bar can be controlled by the experimenter. The animal would be ambulating in the box and may occasionally press the bar. The rate of occasional pressing is used to calculate operant level of bar pressing. One can then begin to reward the animal for bar pressing and see that measure climb as an index of learning. Placing the animal back in that box after a delay and examining how quickly it relearns Inhibitors,research,lifescience,medical to press the bar is an index of memory. The light can be used for discrimination learning (eg, reward bar presses only when the light is on). Research using the operant conditioning Inhibitors,research,lifescience,medical paradigm has unraveled many principles

of learning and memory. It has related the rate of learning a wide range of operant behaviors across animals and humans, discovering markedly selleck chem Dovitinib analogous laws of learning and memory that operate across species. For example, reinforcement strength, frequency, Inhibitors,research,lifescience,medical and predictability have similar effects in worms, pigeons, rodents, and humans. Even some “paradoxical” effects in animals have found immediate translation into human research on learning and memory. For example, in 1908 Yerkes and Dodson

published a study in mice, where they related the strength of a negatively reinforcing stimulus (electric shock) to the speed of avoidance learning.1 They found, as expected, that mice will learn more quickly to avoid moderately strong shocks than mild shocks. Indeed, learning to avoid a moderate shock Inhibitors,research,lifescience,medical took scientific assays between one and two trials. Counterintuitively, however, the strongest shock did not further improve speed of learning but instead slowed it down. This inverted-U relationship between intensity of reinforcement stimulus and what Yerkes and Dodson called “rapidity of habit formation” motivated multiple studies in humans, establishing an inverted-U relationship between anxiety and Entinostat rate of learning and memory.2-5 This work has led to many insights on the nature of learning and memory. For example, it was discovered quite early that sensorimotor skills are learned differently from more complex cognitive functions (see Bell, 1950),6 and quantitative models have been proposed that integrate classical and operant conditioning parameters to account for learning and memory (eg, the Hull-Spence theoryrelating excitatory potential to drive and habit strength and the Estes stimulus sampling theory).

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

*

You may use these HTML tags and attributes: <a href="" title=""> <abbr title=""> <acronym title=""> <b> <blockquote cite=""> <cite> <code> <del datetime=""> <em> <i> <q cite=""> <strike> <strong>